8月1号进行了八月初的第一场雅思的考试,相信大家对真题以及答案会非常的感兴趣、今天就由出国留学网的小编为大家介绍2020年8月1日雅思阅读考试真题答案。
一、考题解析
P1 土地沙漠化
P2 澳大利亚的鹦鹉
P3 多重任务
二、名师点评
1.8月份首场考试的难度总体中等,有出现比较多的配对题,没有出现Heading题,其余主要以常规的填空,判断和选择题为主。文章的话题和题型搭配也是在剑桥真题中都有迹可循,所以备考重心依然还是剑桥官方真题。
2. 整体分析:涉及环境类(P1)、动物类(P2)、社科类(P3)。
本次考试的P2和P3均为旧题。P2是动物类的话题,题型组合为:段落细节配对+单选+summary填空,难度中等。题型上也延续19年的出题特点,出现配对题,考察定位速度和准确度。P3也出现了段落细节配对,主要是段落细节配对+单选+判断。三种题型难度中等,但是文章理解起来略有难度。
3. 部分答案及参考文章:
Passage 1:土地沙漠化
题型及答案待确认
Passage 2:澳大利亚的鹦鹉
题型:段落细节配对+单选+Summary填空
技巧分析:由于段落细节配对是完全乱序出题,在定位时需要先做后面的单选题及填空题,最大化利用已读信息来确定答案,尽量避免重复阅读,以保证充分的做题时间。
文章内容及题目参考:
A 概况,关于一个大的生物种类
B 一些物种消失的原因,题干关键词:an example of one bird species extinct
C 一种鹦鹉不能自己存活,以捕食另一种鸟为生,吃该鸟类的蛋。题干关键词:two species competed at the expense of oneanother
D 吸引鹦鹉的原因以及鹦鹉嘴的特点。题干关键词:analysis of reasons as Australian landscapeattract parrots
E 植物是如何适应鹦鹉。题干关键词:plants attract birds which make the animal adaptto the environment
F 南半球对英语的影响
G 两种鹦鹉从环境改变中获益并存活下来。题干关键词:two species of parrots benefit fromm theenvironment change
H 外来物种及本地鹦鹉
I 鸟类栖息地被破坏以及人类采取的措施
J 作者对于鹦鹉问题的态度
单选题:
why parrots in the whole world are lineal descendants of
选项关键词:continent split from Africa
the writer thinks parrots species beak is for
选项关键词:adjust to their suitable diet
which one is not mentioned
选项关键词:should be frequently maintained
填空题:分布在文章的前两段
one-sixth
16th century
mapmaker
John Gould
Passage 3:多重任务
题型:段落细节配对+单选+判断
参考答案及文章
28 F
29I
30C
31B
32G
33C
34B
35A
36YES
37YES
38NO
39NOT GIVEN
40NO
Passage3: multitasking
Multitasking Debate—Can you do them at the same time?
Talking on the phone while driving isn't the only situationwhere we're worse at multitasking than we might like to think we are. Newstudies have identified a bottleneck in our brains that some say means we arefundamentally incapable of true multitasking. If experimental findings reflectreal-world performance, people who think they are multitasking are probablyjust underperforming in all-or at best, all but one -of their parallelpursuits. Practice might improve your performance, but you will never be asgood as when focusing on one task at a time.
The problem, according to René Marois, a psychologist atVanderbilt University in Nashville, Tennessee, is that there's a sticking pointin the brain. To demonstrate this, Marois devised an experiment to locate nteers watch a screen and when a particular image appears, a red circle,say, they have to press a key with their index finger. Different colouredcircles require presses from different fingers. Typical response time is about half a second, and thevolunteers quickly reach their peak performance. Then they learn to listen todifferent recordings and respond by making a specific sound. For instance, whenthey hear a bird chirp, they have to say "ba"; an electronic soundshould elicit a "ko", and so on. Again, no problem. A normal personcan do that in about half a second, with almost no effort. The trouble comeswhen Marois shows the volunteers an image, then almost immediately plays them asound. Now they're flummoxed. "If you show an image and play a sound atthe same time, one task is postponed," he says. In fact,if the second taskis introduced within the half-second or so it takes to process and react to thefirst, it will simply be delayed until the first one is done. The largestdual-task delays occur when the two tasks are presented simultaneously; delaysprogressively shorten as the interval between presenting the tasks lengthens(See Diagram).
There are at least three points where we seem to getstuck, says Marois. The first is in simply identifying what we're looking can take a few tenths of a second, during which time we are not able tosee and recognise a second item. This limitation is known as the"attentional blink": experiments have shown that if you're watchingout for a particular event and a second one shows up unexpectedly any timewithin this crucial window of concentration, it may register in your visualcortex but you will be unable to act upon it. Interestingly, if you don'texpect the first event, you have no trouble responding to the second. Whatexactly causes the attentional blink is still a matter for debate.
A second limitation is in our short-term visual 's estimated that we can keep track of about four items at a time, fewer ifthey are complex. This capacity shortage is thought to explain, in part, our astonishinginability to detect even huge changes in scenes that are otherwise identical,so-called "change blindness". Show people pairs of near-identicalphotos -say, aircraft engines in one picture have disappeared in the other -andthey will fail to spot the differences (if you don't believe it, check out theclips at /~rensink/flicker/download). Here again, though, thereis disagreement about what the essential limiting factor really is. Does itcome down to a dearth of storage capacity, or is it about how much attention aviewer is paying?
A third limitation is that choosing a response to astimulus -braking when you see a child in the road, for instance,or replyingwhen your mother tells you over the phone that she's thinking of leaving yourdad -also takes brainpower. Selecting a response to one of these things willdelay by some tenths of a second your ability to respond to the other. This iscalled the "response selection bottleneck" theory, first proposed in1952.
Last December, Marois and his colleagues published apaper arguing that this bottleneck is in fact created in two different areas ofthe brain: one in the posterior lateral prefrontal cortex and another in thesuperior medial frontal cortex (Neuron, vol 52, p 1109). They found this byscanning people's brains with functional MRI while the subjects struggled tochoose among eight possible responses to each of two closely timed tasks. Theydiscovered that these brain areas are not tied to any particular sense but aregenerally involved in selecting responses, and they seemed to queue theseresponses when presented with multiple tasks concurrently.
Bottleneck? What bottleneck?
But David Meyer, a psychologist at the University ofMichigan, Ann Arbor, doesn't buy the bottleneck idea. He thinks dual-taskinterference is just evidence of a strategy used by the brain to prioritisemultiple activities. Meyer is known as something of an optimist by his has written papers with titles like "Virtually perfect time-sharing indual-task performance: Uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck"(Psychological Science, vol 12, p101). His experiments have shown that withenough practice -at least 2000 tries -some people can execute two taskssimultaneously as competently as if they were doing them one after the suggests that there is a central cognitive processor that coordinates allthis and, what's more, he thinks it uses discretion: sometimes it chooses todelay one task while completing another.
Even with practice, not all people manage to achieve thisharmonious time-share, however. Meyer argues that individual differences comedown to variations in the character of the processor -some brains are just more"cautious", some more "daring". And despite urban legend,there are no noticeable
differences between men and women. So, according to him,it's not a central bottleneck that causes dual-task interference, but rather"adaptive executive control", which "schedules task processesappropriately to obey instructions about their relative priorities and serialorder".
Marois agrees that practice can sometimes eraseinterference effects. He has found that with just 1 hour of practice each dayfor two weeks, volunteers show a huge improvement at managing both his tasks atonce. Where he disagrees with Meyer is in what the brain is doing to achievethis. Marois speculates that practice might give us the chance to find lesscongested circuits to execute a task -rather like finding trusty back streetsto avoid heavy traffic on main roads -effectively making our response to thetask subconscious. After all, there are plenty of examples of subconsciousmultitasking that most of us routinely manage: walking and talking, eating andreading, watching TV and folding the laundry.
But while some dual tasks benefit from practice, otherssimply do not. "Certain kinds of tasks are really hard to do two atonce," says Pierre Jolicoeur at the University of Montreal, Canada, whoalso studies multitasking. Dual tasks involving a visual stimulus andskeletal-motor response (which he dubs "in the eye and out the hand")and an auditory stimulus with a verbal response ("in the ear and out themouth") do seem to be amenable to practice, he says. Jolicoeur has foundthat with enough training such tasks can be performed as well together asapart. He speculates that the brain connections that they use may be somehowspecial, because we learn to speak by hearing and learn to move by looking. Butpair visual input with a verbal response, or sound to motor, and there's nodramatic improvement. "It looks like no amount of practice will allow youto combine these," he says.
For research purposes, these experiments have to be keptsimple. Real-world multitasking poses much greater challenges. Even the upbeatMeyer is sceptical about how a lot of us live our lives. Instant-messaging andtrying to do your homework? "It can't be done," he says. Conducting ajob interview while answering emails? "There's no way you wind up being asgood." Needless to say, there appear to be no researchers in the area ofmultitasking who believe that you can safely drive a car and carry on a phoneconversation. In fact, last year David Strayer at the University of Utah inSalt Lake City reported that people using cellphones drive no better thandrunks (Human Factors, vol 48, p 381). In another study, Strayer found thatusing a hands-free kit did not improve a driver's response time. He concludedthat what distracts a driver so badly is the very act of talking to someone whoisn't present in the car and therefore is unaware of the hazards facing thedriver.
“No researchers believe it's safe to drive a car andcarry on a phone conversation”
It probably comes as no surprise that, generallyspeaking, we get worse at multitasking as we age. According to Art Kramer atthe University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, who studies how ageing affectsour cognitive abilities, we peak in our 20s. Though the decline is slow throughour 30s and on into our 50s, it is there; and after 55, it becomes moreprecipitous. In one study, he and his colleagues had both young and oldparticipants do a simulated driving task while carrying on a conversation. Hefound that while young drivers tended to miss background changes, older driversfailed to notice things that were highly relevant. Likewise, older subjects hadmore trouble paying attention to the more important parts of a scene than youngdrivers.
It's not all bad news for over-55s, though. Kramer alsofound that older people can benefit from practice. Not only did they learn toperform better, brain scans showed that underlying that improvement was achange in the way their brains become active.
Whileit's clear that practice can often make a difference, especially as we age, thebasic facts remain sobering. "We have this impression of an almightycomplex brain," says Marois, "and yet we have very humbling andcrippling limits." For most of our history, we probably never needed to domore than one thing at a time, he says, and so we haven't evolved to be ableto. Perhaps we will in future, though. We might yet look back one day on peoplelike Debbie and Alun as ancestors of a new breed of true multitaskers.
用户评论
今天上午真难熬,考完雅思考试阅读后感觉脑子一片空白!希望我的阅读速度跟得上题目难度,这样才能拿到理想分数!
有14位网友表示赞同!
8.28 挺不容易的呀,这个日期我报名的是写作和口语,虽然不是阅读考试,但大家加油吧!记得要好好复习各个部分的技巧,祝大家都考得好!
有5位网友表示赞同!
今天刚考完雅思考试阅读,感觉有一些知识点没看明白,是不是其他同学也遇到了类似的问题?希望官方尽快公布真题解析,让我们能及时分析错误并调整学习计划。这期雅思阅读难度怎么样啊,大家觉得?
有6位网友表示赞同!
虽然我考试不是8.28当天,但我也在关注这次雅思阅读考试的趋势,想知道大家的体验如何!希望考过的人早日收到好消息,没过的也不要灰心,继续加油学习,总会取得进步!
有8位网友表示赞同!
每次雅思考试阅读都是我的噩梦……时间不够用,还经常会因为一些细节陷阱而丢分。今天8.28的考试难度怎么样啊?大家分享一下经验吧 感觉阅读理解部分真的很需要技巧和方法...
有14位网友表示赞同!
看到好多人都说8.28雅思阅读挺难的,心里有点紧张!但我还是坚持我的复习计划,相信自己能够拿到理想的分数!
有16位网友表示赞同!
今天考试体验分享:我个人感觉本次雅思阅读难度适中,没有想象中那么困难,掌握一些解题技巧和快速阅读方法还是很关键的。大家加油冲鸭!
有8位网友表示赞同!
我对8.28雅思阅读的真题解析很感兴趣,希望官方能尽快公布,看看这次考试是偏重什么类型的题目,这样我才能更有针对性地进行复习备考! 希望能看到一些专业分析和讲解!
有12位网友表示赞同!
雅思考试阅读虽然很有挑战性,但我觉得只要坚持努力练习,掌握方法就能取得进步!今天8.28的考试,相信很多人都能拿到自己想要的分数!
有9位网友表示赞同!
对于喜欢读书和经常接触学术文献的人来说,雅思阅读应该不难吧?但是这次8.28的考试难度确实有点超出预期,感觉需要更加系统地进行复习!
有14位网友表示赞同!
觉得这次8.28雅思阅读比较考验词汇量和逻辑思维能力,希望未来官方能够提供更多针对性的词库和练习题目,这样备考起来会更加有效率!
有16位网友表示赞同!
我是冲着口语和写作去的,今天不是阅读考试,但希望能看到大家在这方面的经验分享!祝8.28的雅思考试阅读顺利过关!给所有考生的建议:考试前一定要调整好心态,保持良好的睡眠和饮食习惯!
有19位网友表示赞同!
我最近在关注雅思备考,特别是阅读部分。我想知道这次8.28雅思阅读,难度等级会怎样?以及有哪些类型的题目需要注意?希望能从大家的分享里获得一些宝贵信息!
有6位网友表示赞同!
8.28这个日期的考生真的很拼啊!阅读考试确实很考验耐心和速度,希望大家都能顺利完赛并拿到理想分!还有个问题,这次真题解析官方会什么时候发布呢?
有14位网友表示赞同!
每次参加雅思考试阅读都感觉时间不够用,不知道为什么总是卡在一些细节题上。8.28的考试难度怎么样,希望能从大家的反馈中吸取经验教训!希望大家都能按自己的实力去发挥,不要给自己太大压力!
有8位网友表示赞同!
看到很多朋友都在谈论8.28雅思阅读的难易程度,我也有点好奇,感觉这次考试可能是偏重一些长篇阅读文章?还有,有没有人知道这次考试题目中有哪些特别需要注意的部分? 期待大家的分享和解答!
有8位网友表示赞同!
我是准备在10月参加雅思考试 ,现在也在积极复习阅读部分。听说是8.28的考试难度比较高,所以想要提前了解一下相关信息,看看大家是怎么应付这次考试的?希望能够找到一些针对性强的学习方法!
有9位网友表示赞同!